CHAPTER 3 Recognising and bridging gaps
theory, research and practice in clinical education
Current conceptualisations and theoretical perspectives
It is difficult to define clinical education succinctly, certainly difficult to find a definition that everyone can accept, and even more difficult to reach consensus about its goals and purpose. There are many reasons for this but one of the most fundamental is epistemological; that is, to do with how knowledge and learning are conceptualised. Essentially clinical education is concerned with questions such as: What is the nature of clinical knowledge? How does an individual come to ‘possess’ it? How can they be helped in this process? How can we all know that a professional does ‘possess’ the requisite knowledge? Definitions of clinical education and statements about its goals and purposes contain implicit answers (or assumptions) to or about these questions. This section focuses on aspects of two theoretical perspectives, sociocultural and cognitive psychology, which I think can help us to both recognise and bridge some of the gaps in current research and practice in clinical education.
In most healthcare professional education research learning is usually investigated using individualistic psychological understandings and explanations of learning (Swanick 2005). That is, learning is understood as a process in which knowledge is somehow transmitted by the teacher and acquired by the learner. The research focus is usually on either the learner or the teacher, sometimes on the dyad, but generally little attention is paid to context. This is in contrast to much other education research that is predicated on sociocultural theories of learning in which learning is understood as a process of participation in activities which are situated in social and cultural contexts. Anna Sfard (1998) offered a useful analysis of understandings about learning by arguing that there are two current metaphors for learning—acquisition and participation—which guide learners, teachers and researchers. Learning, teaching and research are heavily influenced by whichever metaphor is used (often implicitly). She traces the acquisition metaphor back to Plato, while the participation metaphor has developed more recently in response to explanatory weaknesses in approaches that consider learning as a process of acquisition. Neither metaphor completely explains learning on its own, although it is clear that participation is a crucial but under used concept for clinical education.
Cognitive psychology perspectives on learning
Cognitive psychology research has demonstrated that by learning through participating in routine tasks and activities, concepts and activities are transformed into skilled performance that does not require conscious thought (compilation). Clinicians (and students) develop mental representations of cases—‘illness scripts’—and their clinical reasoning appears to involve both analytical and non-analytical processes (Eva 2005) (see Ch 7). New learning and/or transfer is necessary to perform new and/or non-routine tasks and has been the subject of much debate (for example, Norman et al 2005, Colliver 2004). There is agreement that success in solving one clinical problem does not predict success at solving another, even related, problem and there is recognition that this is due to context specificity. However while this recognition has influenced current assessment practices, it seems to have had less impact on clinical education more generally. One of the main ‘messages’ of this chapter is that it is necessary to develop a much fuller understanding and recognition of contextual factors in order to develop more effective clinical education practices and research.
Sociocultural perspectives
Sociocultural perspectives understand learning to result from individuals constructing their own knowledge as a result of participating in sociocultural activity, for example, a healthcare student undertaking a clinical placement. Learning is situated (that is, contextual, dependent on the social and physical environment) and opportunities to participate—legitimate peripheral participation (Lave & Wenger 1991)—are therefore essential for learning. However these opportunities (‘affordances’) are dependent on many factors including hierarchies, acceptability, personal relationships and workplace culture, and so may not be equal (Billet 2001). Learning is usually understood as a form of internalisation in more individual psychological explanations but Rogoff (1995, Rogoff et al 1995) argues that change resulting from participating in an activity (which the authors call ‘appropriation’) is a process of transformation and not simply internalisation.
There are weaknesses in both perspectives. Cognitive psychology does not fully explain how knowledge is sourced, represented or constructed or how social practices influence this construction, while sociocultural theories do not completely account for the construction of different types of knowledge or how it is used (Billet 1998). So, in clinical education, cognitive psychology can account for aspects of the development of diagnostic skills, for example, but not how they are influenced by different settings and interactions. Conversely, sociocultural theories do not fully address how underpinning knowledge (and its ‘accuracy’ or otherwise) about bodily systems, for example, are used in activities although they do address how participation in activity influences learning. Both cognitive psychology and sociocultural perspectives understand (albeit with different emphases) knowledge acquisition as active and interpretative. Learning is understood more as a process of becoming in sociocultural theories, while in cognitive psychology perspectives it is more understood as a process of making meaning. Arguably, cognitive psychology privileges learning as a process of acquisition and sociocultural theories as a process of participation. Clearly how learning (and knowledge) is understood will determine both how it is investigated and how clinical education is delivered. I am trying to develop a research perspective that can explore and understand learning in clinical settings as involving both processes of becoming and making meaning.
Current empirical work
Research on clinical education encompasses assessment, clinical skills, communication skills, clinical teaching, supervision, community-based education, clinical reasoning, professionalism and many other topics. However, it is often either completely atheoretical or uses a concept (for example, learning styles, reflection, attitudes) without any justification of the use of that particular conceptual framework, underpinning assumptions are unquestioned and there is no acknowledgement of associated theoretical problems. Essentially much clinical education research is concerned with descriptive answers to the question ‘What works?’ Unless there is some understanding of how or why it ‘works’ such research is not helpful in developing deeper or more complex understanding about learning in clinical settings. Cook et al (2008) reviewed 105 studies describing medical ‘education experiments’ and found that 75 (72%) were justification studies (did it work?), 17 (16%) were descriptive (what was done), and only 13 (12%) were concerned with ‘why or how did it work?’ (clarification studies in their terminology). This review is particularly interesting because the authors are writing from within a scientific paradigm and have produced a similar critique to that developed from a more social sciences perspective. Both critiques centre on the problem that too many studies lack a theoretical framework and do not build on or address previous work (for example, Govaerts et al 2007, Kuper et al 2007, van der Vleuten & Schurwith 2005).
There is also a vast body of relevant social science research, particularly in education, that is largely ignored and often results in replication of research and/or debates that have already happened in other academic disciplines. However, I think that it can also be argued, at least to some extent, that other academic disciplines could benefit from considering aspects of clinical education research, but that would be a different chapter!
Existing work in social sciences
Janet Hyde (2005) examined 46 meta-analyses of research into gender differences in psychology. She found that 78% of reported gender differences were small or close to zero; the exceptions were throwing (particularly after puberty), and some measures of aspects of sexuality and physical aggression. However, in medicine, there are persistent ideas and research reports which suggest there are gender differences in motivations and attitudes, differences in academic performance, differences in clinical skills and communication skills, and even that women practice medicine differently. Our literature review (Kilminster et al 2007a) found very little evidence to support such arguments and assumptions; there is a vast amount of work that often produces conflicting findings. We did find problems with some of the research methodologies, frequent failure to report effect sizes, and, of course, there is also publication bias against non-significant findings.
It is essential to remember that ‘[h]ow we interpret data about the relative educational performance of different groups will depend on our research focus, and on our assumptions about the social and psychological processes involved’ (Hammersley 2001, p 293). Furthermore, there is a strong argument that such research can be detrimental for women. Rosemary Pringle (1998) noted the trend in medical culture to exaggerate the ‘feminine’ qualities of women in communication, empathy and care; if women are considered to be good empathetic communicators this may confer short-term advantages over their male counterparts. However this may be a ‘double-edged sword’ that will ultimately restrict women to successful practice only as the ‘new human face of a humbler form of medical practice’. Similarly, in the conclusion to her review of meta-analyses (2005) Janet Hyde argued that ‘over inflated claims of gender differences … cause harm in numerous realms’ (p 590) and, furthermore, such claims are also not consistent with the scientific data.
Research that aims to discern essential gender differences is often predicated upon unquestioned essentialist assumptions about traits and complementary skills that justify and reinforce women’s subordinate position. Furthermore, the cultures and practices of all health professions are interrelated with the social contexts in which healthcare is delivered, so approaches aimed at isolating one factor, such as gender and communication skills, are likely to fail to identify, or explain, the complexities involved. In medicine, there is a lack of research and analysis into how male doctors are contributing to modern medicine that is indicative of the historically masculine context in which medicine is embedded—this is demonstrated by frequent reference to the ‘feminisation’ of medicine. It is female doctors who are researched as the ‘other’, whereas male doctors receive little attention. In the social sciences, gender is generally understood as a relation rather than a trait. Gender relations are maintained by the interaction of various processes at different levels; they are not fixed. Therefore future research will be best served not by assuming; or looking for sex differences, but by examining where gender becomes relevant and impacts on education, training, practice and career paths in healthcare.